KHO THƯ VIỆN 🔎

Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables

➤  Gửi thông báo lỗi    ⚠️ Báo cáo tài liệu vi phạm

Loại tài liệu:     PDF
Số trang:         44 Trang
Tài liệu:           ✅  ĐÃ ĐƯỢC PHÊ DUYỆT
 













Nội dung chi tiết: Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables

Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables

l-l-SI.VII.RCOGNITIONCognition 72 (1999) 1-44Default nominal inflection in Hebrew: evidence for mental variablesIris Berent4*, Steven Pinker1’. Joseph

Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables h Shimronc’Department of Psychology, Florida Atlantic University, 777 Glades Road.P.O. Bas 3091. Boca Raton. FL 33431 0991, USA1Department of Brain an

d Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology Cambridge.. MA. Z.'X-f’School of Education, University of Haifa, Harla, IsraelReceived 14 Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables

January 1999: accepted 13 April 1999AbstractAccording to The ’word rille’ account, regular inflection is computed by a default. symbolic process, wher

Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables

eas irregular inflection is achieved by associative memory. Conversely, pattern-associalor accounts attribute both regular and irregular inflection to

l-l-SI.VII.RCOGNITIONCognition 72 (1999) 1-44Default nominal inflection in Hebrew: evidence for mental variablesIris Berent4*, Steven Pinker1’. Joseph

Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables kens tend to form tight, well-defined phonological clusters (e.g. siiiỊĩ-saiiỊĩ. ring-rangi. whereas regular forms are diffusely distributed throughou

t the phonological space. Ulis distributional asymmetry is necessary and sufficient for the acquisition of a regular default. Hebrew nominal inflectio Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables

n challenges this account. We demonstrate that Hebrew speakers use The regular masculine inflection as a default despite the overlap in the distributi

Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables

on of regular and irregular Hebrew masculine nouns, specifically. Experiment 1 demonstrates that regular inflection is productively applied Io novel n

l-l-SI.VII.RCOGNITIONCognition 72 (1999) 1-44Default nominal inflection in Hebrew: evidence for mental variablesIris Berent4*, Steven Pinker1’. Joseph

Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables similarity to stored irregular tokens. Experiment 2 establishes the generality of The regular default for novel words that are phonologically idiosync

ratic. Experiment 3 demonstrates that Hebrew speakers assign the default regular inflection Io borrowings and names that are identical to existing irr Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables

egular nouns. The existence of default inflection in Hebrew is incompatible with the distributional asymmetry hypothesis. Our findings also lend no su

Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables

pport for a type-frequency account, rhe convergence of the circumstances triggering default inflection in Hebrew. German and English suggests that the

l-l-SI.VII.RCOGNITIONCognition 72 (1999) 1-44Default nominal inflection in Hebrew: evidence for mental variablesIris Berent4*, Steven Pinker1’. Joseph

Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables Corresponding author. Fax: +1-561-2972160E mail address'. iberent@fau edu (1. Berent)0010-0277 99 s - see front matter * 1999 Elsevier Science B V. A

ll rights reservedPI1: S0010-0277(99100027-Xhttps://khothuvien.cori!2I Betent et al. /Cognition 72 (1999/1-441. IntroductionThe study of inflectional Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables

morphology has been the subject of a fierce controversy between symbolic and associative theories of cognition. Common to both accounts is the proposa

Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables

l that irregular inflection (e.g. go-uenr. mouse-mice) is achieved by associative memory. The center of debate concerns regular inflection (e.g. like-

l-l-SI.VII.RCOGNITIONCognition 72 (1999) 1-44Default nominal inflection in Hebrew: evidence for mental variablesIris Berent4*, Steven Pinker1’. Joseph

Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables ns. Pattern-associator accounts (e.g. Rumelhart and McClelland. 1986: MacWhinney and Leinbach. 1991; Plunkett and Marchman. 1991; Daugherty and Seiden

berg. 1992; Plunkett and Marchman. 1993; Hare and Elman. 1995: Hare et al., 1995). thus, attribute both regular and irregular inflection to an associa Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables

tive process. The representation of regular words IS indistinguishable from irregular words. All words are represented solely by their phonological, s

Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables

emantic and orthographic features. Variables, such as noun or verb, are eliminated from mental representations. Regular inflection is thus largely exp

l-l-SI.VII.RCOGNITIONCognition 72 (1999) 1-44Default nominal inflection in Hebrew: evidence for mental variablesIris Berent4*, Steven Pinker1’. Joseph

Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables s regular inflection as a symbolic process. Symbolic processes operate over variables and are blind to the contents of specific tokens (Fodor and Pyly

shyn, 1988). Consequently, regular inflection applies across the board, regardless of the target’s idiosyncratic features. In addition, regular inflec Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables

tion serves as a default: it applies to any target that fails to activate stored associations by the ‘elsewhere condition’. An ‘elsewhere condition’ i

Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables

s the application of a general linguistic process upon the failure to trigger a more specific process (Kiparsky. 1973). The conditions for activating

l-l-SI.VII.RCOGNITIONCognition 72 (1999) 1-44Default nominal inflection in Hebrew: evidence for mental variablesIris Berent4*, Steven Pinker1’. Joseph

Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables ation of any canonical verb stem). A failure to activate irregular inflection thus triggers the regular default.There is substantial empirical support

for the view of regular inflection as a default. For instance, regular inflection is assigned to borrowings, names and denominals, all failing to tri Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables

gger stored associations due to their lack of a canonical root (Kim et al., 1991, 1994; Marcus et al.. 1995). Likewise, regular inflection applies to

Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables

non-words that are dissimilar to English verbs, hence, are unlikely to activate similar stored irregular tokens (Prasada and Pinker, 1993). In both ca

l-l-SI.VII.RCOGNITIONCognition 72 (1999) 1-44Default nominal inflection in Hebrew: evidence for mental variablesIris Berent4*, Steven Pinker1’. Joseph

Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables ction to non-words that are dissimilar to existing regular verbs does not differ from non-words that are highly similar to familiar regular verbs (Pra

sada and Pinker, 1993). Conversely, regular inflection is observed for borrowings, names and denonnnals that are highlyWe use die term 'pattern associ Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables

ator' to refer to die class of cognitive models that eliminate mental variables. Our criticism of die ’pattern-associator- hypothesis should not be eq

Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables

uated with a criticism of connectionist formalism. As we next explain, connectionism is fully compatible with symbolic accounts of cognition. Likewise

l-l-SI.VII.RCOGNITIONCognition 72 (1999) 1-44Default nominal inflection in Hebrew: evidence for mental variablesIris Berent4*, Steven Pinker1’. Joseph

Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables sis strictly concerns mental representations. not their implementations (for a similar distinction. see Pinker and Prince. 1988; .Marcus et al.. 1995;

Marcus. 1998b. 1999).I. Bet ent el at. /Cognition 72 (1999) 1-113similar, or even identical to stored irregular words (Kim et al., 1991.1994: Marcus Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables

et al.. 1995).Despite the strong empirical support for the existence of a default inflection, controversy still remains regarding its source. Accordin

Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables

g to the symbolic view, the wide variety of circumstances resulting in default inflection indicates that regular inflection operates over mental varia

l-l-SI.VII.RCOGNITIONCognition 72 (1999) 1-44Default nominal inflection in Hebrew: evidence for mental variablesIris Berent4*, Steven Pinker1’. Joseph

Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables like. explain. refrigerate. Variables define equivalence classes: they assign a uniform representation to all the tokens they enumerate, ignoring the

ir individual idiosyncrasies. It is the uniform representation of all nouns (or verbs) by a single label that explains the generality of regular infle Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables

ction and its insensitivity to token-specific features. Likewise, it is the representation of the grammatical category of a ‘root' which explains the

Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables

blocking of irregular inflection for denominate, names and borrowings, despite their strong resemblance to stored irregular tokens. The appeal to vari

l-l-SI.VII.RCOGNITIONCognition 72 (1999) 1-44Default nominal inflection in Hebrew: evidence for mental variablesIris Berent4*, Steven Pinker1’. Joseph

Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables . The emergence of a regular default may be adequately explained by an associative process (e.g. Rumelhart and McClelland, 1986; Seidenberg. 1987. 199

7; Seidenberg and McClelland, 1989; Elman. 1993; Hare and Elman. 1995; Elman et al., 1996; Plant et al., 1996; Rueckl et al.. 1997).The present resear Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables

ch examines whether default inflection appeals to mental variables. We first review two associative explanations for the emergence of default inflecti

Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables

on. We then present a new challenge to the associative account: Hebrew nominal inflection. We describe the distributional properties of Hebrew nouns a

l-l-SI.VII.RCOGNITIONCognition 72 (1999) 1-44Default nominal inflection in Hebrew: evidence for mental variablesIris Berent4*, Steven Pinker1’. Joseph

Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables f default inflectionAssociative theories of cognition have proposed two accounts for the emergence of default inflection: the type frequency and the d

istributional asymmetry hypotheses. According to the type-frequency account (e.g. Rumelhart and McClelland. 1986; Plunkett and Marchman, 1991; Daugher Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables

ty and Seidenberg, 1992; Plunkett and Marchman. 1993; Bybee. 1995), the role of regular inflection as a default is due to the ubiquity of regular type

Default nominal inflection in hebrew evidence for mental variables

s in the language. In modern English, for example, regular inflection applies to the majority of verbs in the language. Thus, the probability that a n

Gọi ngay
Chat zalo
Facebook